eISSN: 2300-6722
ISSN: 1899-1874
Medical Studies/Studia Medyczne
Current issue Archive Manuscripts accepted About the journal Supplements Editorial board Abstracting and indexing Subscription Contact Instructions for authors Publication charge Ethical standards and procedures
Editorial System
Submit your Manuscript
3/2022
vol. 38
 
Share:
Share:
Original paper

Assessment of selected variables of functional capacity in three-generational family households

Tomasz Sikorski
1
,
Szymon Krupnik
2
,
Szymon Pasiut
3
,
Magdalna Wasik
1
,
Jarosław Andrychowski
1
,
Marek R. Żak
1

  1. Institute of Health Sciences, Collegium Medicum, Jan Kochanowski University, Kielce, Poland Head of the Institute: Prof. Edyta Suliga PhD
  2. Symmetry, Medical Rehabilitation Centre, Sosnowiec, Poland
  3. Faculty of Motor Rehabilitation, University School of Physical Education, Krakow, Poland Head of the Faculty: Prof. Piotr Mika PhD
Medical Studies/Studia Medyczne 2022; 38 (3): 182–189
Online publish date: 2022/09/30
Article file
Get citation
 
PlumX metrics:
 

Introduction

Generally acknowledged advances in medical science, in conjunction with the much-enhanced public awareness of appreciable benefits offered by a healthy lifestyle, have been instrumental in the continually swelling numbers of persons over 65 years of age, consequently impacting the Aging Index [1]. The Aging Index, an indicator of a generational relationship between grandparents and their grandchildren [2], is construed as the number of persons aged ≥ 65 years per 100 persons < 14 years of age. As of 1990, seniors have accounted for 10% of Poland’s population. This in turn is going to be reflected through the increasingly long intergenerational relationships between grandparents and their grandchildren. Obviously, being confronted with the seniors’ appreciably extended lifespan calls for specific measures to be adopted, aimed specifically at promoting successful ageing, to have this issue addressed effectively [2].
The aging process causes a decrease in individual functional capacity. One of the key determinants of individual fitness is one’s regular gait. Gait is perceived as a comprehensively structured skill because its overall efficacy is directly dependent upon effective orchestration of many different systems within one’s body. A disorder or damage sustained by a single one of them is bound to adversely affect this fitness variable, as well as deteriorate even further, as a particular dysfunction remains unaddressed [2, 3]. The key variables of gait, i.e. speed, length, and width of the stride, and cadence prove most helpful in assessing individual functional capacity [3–5].
It should also be noted at this juncture that gait is by no means a totally automatic activity. This assertion is corroborated through the execution of pertinent dual-task tests [6], subject to manifest control by one’s cognitive abilities [6]. Cognitive processes are responsible for the exchange and flow of vital data between an individual and an immediate environment by way of receiving, storing, and processing information, to be subsequently transformed into an adequate bodily response [7–9]. Consequently, the cognitive component controlling any dual-task gait effectively stands for its correct execution. Gait affects the correlations between the key performance variables of all generations within a single family [10]. The outcomes of the studies pursued on twin siblings indicate that genetic factors account for 15–51% of the variance in individual gait speed [11].
The genes affect individual physical activity, both directly and indirectly, by controlling one’s energy expenditure [12]. In terms of applicable scientific constraints, 2 major lines of regulation involving genes are distinguished. The first one refers to the protein encoding genes that directly regulate one’s energy expenditure, e.g. proteins from the UCP group (uncoupling proteins). Proteins in this group dissipate energy released during respiratory substrates in the mitochondria, as opposed to the ones concentrating energy like ATP synthesis [13]. Polymorphic versions in the coding region of these proteins are characterised by lower activity and thus higher energy expenditure and higher levels of daily physical activity [12].
The other group is made up of the metabolic proteins linked to endocrine functions. The mechanism of hormonal control exerted over one’s physical activity may involve a correlation of physical activity with the level of a specific hormone, or an impaired activity of its receptor. This may cause an appreciable drop in the normal concentration of the hormone in the blood stream [13].
Individual muscle strength is part of one’s hereditary characteristics [9]. Individual muscle strength is accounted for by the actual location of the genes in one of 16 human genomes. Different genes are responsible for a different type of strength in different muscles [14, 15]. The genes responsible for the hand grip strength are TRIM63 and FBXO63 [10, 14, 15]. One’s isometric strength is inherited to a greater extent than muscular endurance [16].
Apart from the genetic aspects, functional fitness may be determined through the assessment of dual-task activities. Available studies indicate that dual-task motor and motor-cognitive activities contribute to enhancement of standing postural control in older adults, as well as boosting their cognitive functions whilst also remaining predictive of the ageing process at large [17].
The study aimed to examine the way in which individual functional fitness was shaped in the multi-generational families, primarily in terms of individual balance and gait.

Study aims and research questions

The study aimed to assess the selected variables of functional capacity in multi-generational families, principally balance and gait speed, whilst probing the intergenerational ties relative to the progression of the aging process, as expressed through the differences in gait speed between the 1st and the 3rd generation.
The following research questions were therefore posed:
1. Do the subjects differ in functional performance, as expressed through individual gait, dual-task motor and motor-cognitive activities, balance, gait speed, and the speed of processing complex information?
2. Does the progression of the ageing process, as expressed in gait speed, indicate a correlation between respective generations within the same family?
3. How significantly is the motor variable in the first generation related to the difference in gait speed between the first and third generations?
The following research hypotheses were posed:
1. The subjects (between respective generations) differ in their level of functional fitness, as expressed in gait, dual-task motor and motor-cognitive activities, balance, gait speed, and speed of processing complex information.
2. The speed of the ageing process expressed in gait speed indicates a correlation between respective generations within the same family.
3. A motor variable increasing in the first generation indicates an increase in the difference in gait speed between the first and the third generations.

Material and methods

The study protocol covered 40 individuals from 3 ge­- nerations within the same family households. Generation I comprised the individuals aged 62–72 years, Generation II – the ones aged 41–50 years, and Generation III – those aged 20–27 years (Table 1).
The inclusion criterion was that all 3 generations should be from within a single family household. The exclusion criteria comprised making regular use of orthopaedic supplies, obesity, and having sustained an incidental fall within the last 12 months.
The study protocol embraced 6 young women and 2 young men, 6 adult women, and 2 adult men, 6 senior women, and 2 senior men. Altogether, 16 individuals were dropped from the study protocol outcomes at the results calculation stage due to their non-participation in the Generation I study within their family households.
Written informed consent to attend the study protocol was obtained from each individual.
The Timed Up and Go (TUG) test – preparation for the test entails setting up a chair and demarcating a distance of 3 m in a straight line from it. The starting and finishing position is the subject’s sitting down on the chair. At the investigator’s command (“off you go, start”), the subject gets up from the chair, walks the pre-determined distance, turns around by 180° to return to the starting position, which marks the end of the test. The duration of the completed task is timed by a stopwatch. This test is meant to assess individual gait and balance [18].
The Timed Up and Go (TUGMAN) test – an additional manual task is added to the basic version of the TUG test. Apart from the gait, the subject is to focus on holding a cup of water in his/her hand throughout the test. This test is meant to assess individual dual-tasking capacity [19].
The Timed Up and Go Cognitive (TUGCOG) test – a modified TUG test in which a mental (cognitive) task is added. The subject, apart from the gait itself, is to focus additionally on, e.g., counting down from 100 in units of 7. This test is meant to assess individual gait under the dual-task conditions [20].
Single Leg Stance Open Eyes (SLSOP) test – the subject tries to stand and maintain balance on one leg for as long as possible, with the eyes open, without making use of any supports. This test takes place on a flat surface, and the investigator makes use of a stopwatch to time the subject’s ability to maintain a one-legged stand. This test is meant to assess the static body balance [21]. Single Leg Stance Close Eyes (SLSCL) – the performance of this test is similar to the above-referenced version, the only difference being that the subject makes an attempt at maintaining a one-legged stand with his/her eyes closed for as long as possible [21].
The Trail Making Test part B (TMT-B) test – the subject joins the circles featured on a sheet of paper with the letters and numbers inside. The 13 numbers and 12 letters must be connected alternately in numerical and alphabetical order, respectively. The test is timed with a stopwatch, and the subjects are notified of any errors they may have made and are required to correct them, without stopping the timing. The test is meant to assesses the visuospatial aspects of individual memory, attention span, interhemispheric functions, and processing of complex information [22, 23].
The 5-Metre Gait Speed Test – the subject is to walk a distance of 5 metres in a straight line, along a flat surface, at his/her natural gait speed, which is timed with a stopwatch by the investigator. The test is meant to assess individual speed and quality of gait [24].
Dependent variables: Difference in gait speed between respective generations and performance in performing dual-task activities between respective generations. Independent variables: TUG, TUGMAN, TUGCOG, SLSOP, SLSCL, TMT-B, and gait speed.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out with the aid of the Statistica 12 PL software package. The normality of the distribution of the variables under study was assessed by the Shapiro-Wilk test (n < 100). Descriptive statistics were completed. Pertinent percentage distributions of specific response data, along with the actual number of respondents, were calculated in terms of the qualitative statistics. As far as the variables of quantitative nature were concerned, the mean values or medians and the standard deviations were calculated. Statistical hypotheses were tested for quantitative data with the aid of ANOVA analysis of variance.
The correlation between the variables was tested by making use of the correlation of multiple regression model. The  level was set at 0.05.

Results

The Generation I subjects completed the motor TUG (dual-task) test slower by 86%, as compared to the ones from Generation III. The Generation II subjects, on the other hand, were also slower, i.e. by 39%, as compared to the Generation III ones. Their scores were higher than those achieved by Generation II, i.e. by 20%, and the ones achieved by Generation III, i.e. by 65%. The best scores in the standing on one leg with eyes closed tests were claimed by Generation III. They outperformed Generation I by 375%, and Generation II by 96%. Individuals from Generation I also completed the 5-metre gait speed test at 41% lower than the individuals from Generation III, and 18% lower than the individuals from Generation II (Table 2).
Statistical analysis indicated a significant, strong, and negative correlation between the gait speed of Generation I and Generation II (r = –0.81, p = 0.014). This means that if the TUG score increased in Generation I, it decreased in Generation II (Table 3).
The inter-generation difference in individual gait speed was the highest between Generation I and Generation III. This difference significantly, positively, and very strongly correlated with the scope of dual-task motor-cognitive activities. This test (TUGCOG) accounted for the variability of the difference between GS I and GS III in 87.5% (p < 0.05) (Table 4).
The graph shows the distribution of the test results within respective family households. Sixteen family households were investigated, 8 of whom furnished complete results across all 3 generations. The results for 2 generations only were collected from 8 family households. As may easily be ascertained from the graph (Figure 1), there is a combination of data between Generation II and Generation III, whereas in the family households where pertinent data were collected from all 3 generations, it would appear that approx. half of the results are indicative of a significant stratification between Generation I and Generation III. It would also appear that gait speed in the youngest generation was the fastest, remaining on the rise in 4 family households, whilst demonstrating no linkage whatsoever with the gait speed of the fathers or grandfathers (Figure 1).

Discussion

The study aimed to probe the nature of the inter-generational relationship, along with the progression of the ageing process. The study attests to a correlation of gait speed between the 1st and the 2nd generation (strong positive correlation, r = 0.8). Approximately, 69 genes are most likely responsible for encoding the gait speed [25]. At least 3 of them, PRSS16, WDSUB1, and PTPRT, may be expressed by way of enhancing the synaptic signalling, as well as through an effect of neuroplasticity. In the older adults, these genes were not shown to have been expressed [25].
On the other hand, there is a manifest scarcity of reports on this topic, either originating in epigenetics or genetics focused specifically on investigating the issue of gait speed in seniors. Adams et al. demonstrated that heritability was related to individual gait variability (61%), gait rhythm (37%), and the length of the double-pronation phase (32%) because these variables were correlated with body weight and height [26].
Heckerman et al. highlighted the ZNF295 and C2CD2 genes, which indicated the inheritance of gait speed (a 4-metre gait test) [27]. Taking due note of these reports, in conjunction with the authors’ own study results, being clearly indicative of a high correlation between the 1st and the 2nd generation, may offer further credible evidence of the genetic studies at issue. The strongest correlation was demonstrated not to have occurred between the scope of dual-task activities between the 1st and the 2nd generation, but instead between the differences in individual gait speed values between the 1st and the 3rd generation (r = 0.9). In practical terms, this means that high scores achieved within the scope of dual-task motor-cognitive activities would stand to significantly affect this difference.
Consequently, the lower the scores of dual-task activities pursued when young, the smaller would be the difference in gait speed between the youngest and the oldest generation (i.e. 1st and 2nd). None of the studies focused on the dual-task activities, as published to date, have investigated such a linkage, as addressed and diligently probed in the present study.
The outcomes reported by the present study demonstrated the significant differences, as encountered in the test performance time, to be dependent on age. The best results were achieved by the youngest individuals, whereas with each successive generation those values dropped accordingly.
Similar conclusions were reached by Vieira [28], whose study showed a decrease in TUG test scores in each successive age group older by 10 years. The differences in the TUG test scores also varied by gender. Bergland et al. [29] reported that men outperformed women on the TUG test within the 65–80 years age range. Their study also looked at the life expectancy in relation to the quality of performance in the TUG test.
All 3 generations scored below normal in the motor-cognitive TUG test. Those from Generation III were the closest to the American norm (9.82 s), whereas those from Generation I completed the test in almost twice the time indicated by the reference values. The best scores in the standing on one leg with the eyes open test were achieved by the Generation III members.
The follow-up regarding the actual rate of individual ageing, spanning almost 12 years, established a strong association between poor TUG test scores and increased mortality in both sexes. De Buyser et al. [30] came to similar conclusions during a 15-year follow-up, embracing the subjects with a baseline age of 71–81 years. Also, very poor TUG scores were strongly associated with increased mortality over the following 9 years in the study by Tice et al. [31], who investigated subjects with a mean age of 68 years. This test has been proposed as a way of identifying older adults at high risk of adverse health outcomes [32]. The modification of the TUG test by way of adding a cognitive factor is also acknowledged to significantly affect individual gait. The results of the present study indicate that dual-task activity pursued in conjunction with the TUGCOG test made the test scores significantly worse across all age groups.
Similar conclusions were reached by Allali et al. [33], who introduced a second task apart from gait (i.e. subtracting from a specific number by a fixed amount) during the execution of the TUG test. Dual-task conditions were used specifically to investigate the involvement of the cortical level in individual gait control [34]. Changes in dual-task gait were related to the actual performance of the executive function, the level of difficulty of a specific task assigned to the subject, or the articulo-motor components of a walking-related task [34].
During dual-task gait, a lack of gait deceleration, or even acceleration, may be encountered in some cases. This may be due to the prioritization of gait or cognitive functions during the execution of the test. The focusing of the subject’s attention on a motor task (walking) or on a cognitive task (subtracting from a given number by a fixed value) is established to affect the actual outcome of the TUGCOG test [35].
The outcomes yielded by the present study highlighted the likelihood of heritability of cognitive prioritisation among the descendants, as a slow-down of individual gait during the execution of the dual-task motor-cognitive test was experienced by all the subjects originating from the same families.
Oh et al. [36] conducted a study assessing the static balance of the individuals within the age range spanning 20 years, i.e. up to late adulthood. Their study reported the scores obtained in this test by the individuals aged 20–40 years to be twice as good, as compared to the ones achieved by the subjects over 65 years of age. The authors’ own research attested to the scores achieved by the study subjects from the 3rd generation (their grandchildren, great grandchildren) to have been almost twice as good as the ones obtained by the 1st generation (their grandparents).
In view of a small study group, and no opportunity to pursue genetic testing, further studies are planned shortly, with a view gaining as many insights as possible into the issue of heritability of individual gait speed, as well as into key characteristics of the ageing process within members of the same family households.

Conclusions

The respective study groups differed in the score values of the following variables: gait speed, dual-task activities, balance level, and the speed of processing complex information. The 3rd generation of the family boasted the highest scores.
A strong, negative correlation was encountered in the gait speed scores between the 1st and the 2nd generations of the same family. Each successive generation underperformed in gait speed, relative to the previous generation.
An increase in the motor variable value in the 1st generation was also established to translate directly into an increase in the difference between an individual gait speed within the 1st and the 3rd generation. Gait speed differences established between the 1st and the 3rd generation proved directly dependent upon the actual scope of dual-task activities assigned to them.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

1. Kaźmierczak U, Radzimińska A, Dźierżanowski M, Bułatowicz I, Strojek K, Srokowski G, Zukow W. The benefits of regular physical activity for the elderly. J Educ Health Sport 2015; 5: 56-68.
2. Kaczmarzyk M, Trafiałek E. Activating the elderly as a chance for successful aging. Gereontol Pol 2007; 15: 116-118.
3. Steffen TM, Hacker TA, Mollinger L. Age- and gender-related test performance in community-dwelling elderly people: six-minute walk test, berg balance scale, timed up & go test, and gait speeds. Phys Ther 2002; 82: 128-137.
4. Skrzypiec J, Snela S, Bała K, Bazarnik-Mucha K, Dudek J, Szczepanik M, Szymczyk D. The range of changes in time and space parameters of gait in a homogeneous age group. Young Sport Sci Ukraine 2012; 4: 143-148.
5. Zak M, Biskup M, Macek P, Krol H, Krupnik S, Opuch- lik A. Identifying predictive motor factors for falls in post-menopausal breast cancer survivors. PLoS One 2017; 12: e0173970.
6. Beauchet O, Launay C, Annweiler C, Fantino B, Allali G, De Decker L. Physical training-related changes in gait variability while single and dual tasking in older adults: magnitude of gait variability at baseline matters. Eur J Phys Rehab Med 2013; 49: 857-864.
7. Jarosz M, Czubalski K, Heszen-Niejodek I. Medical Psychology. PZWL, Warszawa 1988.
8. ŠvobŠtrac D, Pivac N, Mück-Šeler D. The serotonergic system and cognitive function. Transl Neurosci 2016; 7: 35-49.
9. Łoś R, Latała-Łoś E. Disturbances of the cognitive functions of vehicle drivers in selected diseases. Med Stud 2021; 37: 145-150.
10. Willems SM, Wright DJ, Day FR, Trajanoska K, Joshi PK, Morris JA, Matteini AM, Garton FC, Grarup N, Oskol- kov N, Thalamuthu A, Mangino M, Liu J, Demirkan A, Lek M, Xu L, Wang G, Oldmeadow C, Gaulton KJ, Lotta LA, Miyamoto-Mikami E, Rivas MA, White T, Loh PR, Aadahl M, Amin N, Attia JR, Austin K, Benyamin B, Bra- ge S, Cheng YC, Cięszczyk P, Derave W, Eriksson KF, Eynon N, Linneberg A, Lucia A, Massidda M, Mit- chell BD, Miyachi M, Murakami H, Padmanabhan S, Pan- dey A, Papadimitriou I, Rajpal DK, Sale C, Schnurr TM, Sessa F, Shrine N, Tobin MD, Varley I, Wain LV, Wray NR, Lindgren CM, MacArthur DG, Waterworth DM, McCar- thy MI, Pedersen O, Khaw KT, Kiel DP; GEFOS Any-Type of Fracture Consortium, Pitsiladis Y, Fuku N, Franks PW, North KN, van Duijn CM, Mather KA, Hansen T, Hansson O, Spector T, Murabito JM, Richards JB, Rivadeneira F, Langenberg C, Perry JRB, Wareham NJ, Scott RA. Large-scale GWAS identifies multiple loci for hand grip strength providing biological insights into muscular fitness. Nat Commun 2017; 12: e16015.
11. Ortega-Alonso A, Pedersen NL, Kujala UM, Sipilä S, Törmäkangas T, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M, Rantanen T. A twin study on the heritability of walking ability among older women. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci 2006; 61: 1082-1085.
12. Thorburn AW, Proietto J. Biological determinants of spontaneous physical activity. Obesity Rev 2000; 1: 87-94.
13. Jarmużkiewicz W, Woyda-Płoszczyca A. Mitochondrial uncoupling proteins: regulation and physiological role. Postęp Biochemii 2008; 54: 179-187.
14. Han HQ, Zhou X, Mitch WE, Goldberg AL. Myostatin/active inpathway antagonism: molecularbasis and therapeutic potential. Int J Biochem Cell Biol 2013; 45: 2333-2347.
15. Cohen S, Nathan JA, Goldberg AL. Muscle wasting in disease: molecular mechanisms and promising therapies. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2015; 14: 58-74.
16. Beunen G, Thomis M. Gene driven power athletes? Genetic variation in muscular strength and power. Br J Sports Med 2006; 40: 822-823.
17. Hiyamizu M, Morioka S, Shomoto K, Shimada T. Effects of dual task balance training on dual task performance in elderly people: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehab 2012; 26: 58-67.
18. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 1991; 39: 142-148.
19. Shumway-Cook A, Brauer S, Woollacott M. Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the Timed Up & Go Test. Phys Ther 2000; 80: 896-903.
20. O’Shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R. Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther 2002; 82: 888-897.
21. Cuesta-Vargas A, Giné-Garriga M. Development of a new index of balance in adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities. PLoS One 2014; 9: e96529.
22. Vaucher P, Herzig D, Cardoso I, Herzog MH, Mangin P, Favrat B. The trail making test as a screening instrument for driving performance in older drivers; a research. BMC Geriatrics 2014; 14: 123.
23. Mosiołek A. Methods of researching cognitive functions. Psychiatry 2014; 11: 215-221.
24. Nagano K, Hori H, Muramatsu K. A comparison of at-home walking and 10-meter walking test parameters of individuals with post-stroke hemiparesis. J Phys Ther Sci 2015; 27: 357-359.
25. Ben-Avraham D, Karasik D, Verghese J, Lunetta KL, Smith JA, Eicher JD, Vered R, Deelen J, Arnold AM, Buchman AS, Tanaka T, Faul JD, Nethander M, Fornage M, Adams HH, Matteini AM, Callisaya ML, Smith AV, Yu L, De Jager PL, Evans DA, Gudnason V, Hofman A, Pattie A, Corley J, Launer LJ, Knopman DS, Parimi N, Turner ST, Bandinelli S, Beekman M, Gutman D, Sharvit L, Mooijaart SP, Liewald DC, Houwing-Duistermaat JJ, Ohlsson C, Moed M, Verlinden VJ, Mellström D, van der Geest JN, Karlsson M, Hernandez D, McWhirter R, Liu Y, Thom- son R, Tranah GJ, Uitterlinden AG, Weir DR, Zhao W, Starr JM, Johnson AD, Ikram MA, Bennett DA, Cummings SR, Deary IJ, Harris TB, Kardia SL, Mosley TH, Sri- kanth VK, Windham BG, Newman AB, Walston JD, Davies G, Evans DS, Slagboom EP, Ferrucci L, Kiel DP, Murabito JM, Atzmon G. The complex genetics of gait speed: genome-widemeta-analysis approach. Aging 2017; 9: 209-246.
26. Adams HH, Verlinden VJ, Callisaya ML, van Duijn CM, Hofman A, Thomson R, Uitterlinden AG, Vernooij MW, van der Geest JN, Srikanth V, Ikram MA. Heritability and genome-wide association analyses of human gait suggest contribution of common variants. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci 2016; 71: 740-746.
27. Heckerman D, Traynor BJ, Picca A, Calvani R, Marzetti E, Hernandez D, Nalls M, Arepalli S, Ferruci L, Landi F. Genetic variants associated with physical performance and anthropometry in old age: a genome-wide association study in the ilSIRENTE cohort. Sci Rep 2017; 7: 15879.
28. Vieira WO. Test timed up and go and its correlation with age and functional exercise capacity in asymptomatic women. Fisioterapiaem Movimento 2017; 30: 463-471.
29. Bergland A, Jørgensen L, Emaus N, Strand BH. Mobility as a predictor of all-cause mortality in older men and women: 11.8-year follow-up in the Tromsø study. BMC Health Services Res 2017; 17: 22.
30. De Buyser SL, Petrovic M, Taes YE, Toye KR, Kauf- man JM, Goemaere S. Physical function measurements predict mortality in ambulatory older men. Eur J Clin Investig 2013; 43: 379-386.
31. Tice JA, Kanaya A, Hue T, Rubin S, Buist DS, Lacroix A, Lacey JV Jr, Cauley JA, Litwack S, Brinton LA, Bauer DC. Risk factors for mortality in middle-aged women. JAMA Intern Med 2006; 166: 2469-2477.
32. Savva GM, Donoghue OA, Horgan F, O’Regan C, Cro- nin H, Kenny RA. Using timed up-and-go to identify frail members of the older population. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013; 68: 441-446.
33. Allali G, Laidet M, Armand S, Elsworth-Edelsten C, Assal F, Lalive PH. Stride time variability as a marker for higher level of gait control in multiple sclerosis: its association with fear of falling. J Neural Transmission 2016; 123: 595-599.
34. Allali G, Kressig RW, Assal F, Herrmann FR, Dubost V, Beauchet O. Changes in gait while backward counting in demented older adults with frontal lobe dysfunction. Gait Posture 2007; 26: 572-576.
35. Zak M, Krupnik S, Brola W, Rebak D, Sikorski T, Du- theil F, Andrychowski J, Courteix D. Functional capacity and dual-task cost in the institutionalized older adults, both affected and unaffected by mild cognitive impairment. Eur Rev Aging Phys Act 2021; 18: 16.
36. Oh KY, Kim SA, Lee SY, Lee YS.Comparison of manual balance and balance board tests in healthy adults. Ann Rehab Med 2011; 35: 873-879.
Copyright: © 2022 Jan Kochanowski University in Kielce This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), allowing third parties to copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format and to remix, transform, and build upon the material, provided the original work is properly cited and states its license.
Quick links
© 2024 Termedia Sp. z o.o.
Developed by Bentus.